A cue specifically associated with extinction reduces response recovery in human predictive learning
- A. Matías Gámez 1
- Pedro M. Ogállar 2
- Rodolfo Bernal-Gamboa 3
-
1
Universidad de Córdoba
info
-
2
Universidad de Jaén
info
-
3
Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México
info
ISSN: 1576-8597
Año de publicación: 2022
Volumen: 43
Número: 2
Tipo: Artículo
Otras publicaciones en: Psicológica: Revista de metodología y psicología experimental
Resumen
An experiment evaluated whether a stimulus associated with extinction can attenuate the reinstatement of a previously extinguished predictive learning relationship in humans. Participants learned a specific relationship between two cues (X and Y) and two outcomes (O1 and O2) during the first phase. Throughout extinction, both cues were presented without outcomes. Then, testing was conducted after exposure to the original outcomes. We found a reduction of the reinstatement effect when participants received a cue associated with extinction, but not when testing involved a novel cue. This result indicates that the reductive effect depends on the cue’s specific association with extinction. The findings are consistent with the theoretical view that explains reinstatement as a failure to retrieve the extinction learning.
Referencias bibliográficas
- Bernal-Gamboa, R., Gámez, A. M., & Nieto, J. (2017). Reducing spontaneous recovery and reinstatement of operant performance through extinction-cues. Behavioural Processes, 135, 1-7. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2016.11.010
- Bouton, M. E. (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80–99. http://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.80
- Bouton, M. E. (1994). Conditioning, remembering, and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 219–231. http://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.20.3.219
- Bouton, M. E. (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. In M. E. Bouton & M. S. Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation, and cognition: The functional behaviorism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385-409). American Psychological Association. Bouton, M.E., 2014. Why behavior change is difficult to sustain. Preventive Medicine, 68, 29-36. https://doi.org/36. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2014.06.010
- Bouton, M.E. (2017) Extinction: Behavioral mechanisms and their implications. In: Menzel, R. (ed.), Learning Theory and Behavior, Vol. 1 of Learning and Memory: A Comprehensive Reference, 2nd edition, Byrne, J.H. (ed.). pp. 61–83. Oxford Academic Press.
- Bouton, M.E., & Bolles, R.C. (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation, 10, 445–466. http://doi.org/10.1016/0023-9690(79)90057-2.
- Bouton, M. E., Todd, T. P., Vurbic, D. & Winterbauer, N., 2011. Renewal after the extinction of free operant behavior. Learning & Behavior, 39, 57-67. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-011-0018-6
- Bouton, M. E., & Swartzentruber, D. (1991). Sources of relapse after extinction in Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 123-140. https://doi.org/10.1016/0272-7358(91)90091-8
- Brooks, D. C., & Bouton, M. E. (1994). A retrieval cue for extinction attenuates response recovery (renewal) caused by a return to the conditioning context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 366–379. https://doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.20.4.366
- Brooks, D. C., & Bowker, J. L. (2001). Further evidence that conditioned inhibition is not the mechanism of an extinction cue's effect: A reinforced prevents spontaneous recovery. Animal Learning & Behavior, 29, 381–388. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192903
- Brooks, D.C., & Fava, D. A. (2017). An extinction cue reduces appetitive Pavlovian reinstatement in rats. Learning and Motivation, 58, 59-65. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2017.04.002
- Craske, M.G., Kircanski, K., Zelikowsky, M., Mystkowski, J., Chowdhury, N., & Baker, A. (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 5-27. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2007.10.003
- Craske, M. G., Liao, B., Brown, L., & Vervliet, B. (2012). Role of inhibition in exposure therapy. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 3, 322-345. https://doi.org/10.5127/jep.026511
- Craske, M.G., Treanor, M., Conway, C., Zbozinek, T., & Vervliet, B. (2014). Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 58, 10-23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2014.04.006
- Delamater, A.R. (1997). Selective reinstatement of stimulus-outcome associations. Animal Learning Behavior, 25, 400–412. http://dx.doi.org/10.3758/BF03209847
- Gámez, A. M., & Bernal-Gamboa, R. (2018a). The reinstatement effect in human predictive learning is reduced by extinction reminders. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21, 1-9. http://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2018.53
- Gámez, A. M., & Bernal-Gamboa, R. (2018b). Reinstatement of instrumental actions in humans: possible mechanisms and its implications to prevent it. Acta Psychologica, 183, 29-36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.12.012
- García-Gutiérrez, A., Rosas, J.M. (2003). Context change as the mechanism of reinstatement in causal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 292–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.29.4.292
- García-Gutiérrez, A., Rosas, J. M., & Nelson, J. B. (2005). Extensive interference attenuates reinstatement in human predictive learning. The International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 240-248. Retrieved from: http://pub-eschol-prd-lb-2076919909.us-west-2.elb.amazonaws.com/uc/item/96b647tm
- Haaker, J., Golkar, A., Hermans, D., & Lonsdorf, T.B. (2014). A review on human reinstatement studies: An overview and methodological challenges. Learning and Memory, 21, 424-40. http://dx.doi.org/10.1101/lm.036053.114
- LaBar, K.S., & Phelps, E.A. (2005). Reinstatement of conditioned fear in humans is context dependent and impaired in amnesia. Behavioral Neuroscience, 119, 677–686. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0735-7044.119.3.677
- León, S. P., Abad, M. J.F., & Rosas, J. M. (2011). Context–outcome associations mediate context-switch effects in a human predictive learning task. Learning and Motivation, 42, 84-98. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.10.001
- McConnell, B.L., & Miller, R.R. (2014). Associative accounts of recovery-from-extinction effects. Learning and Motivation, 46, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2014.01.003
- Neumann, D.L. (2008). The Effects of Context Changes on the Reinstatement of Extinguished Conditioned Behavior in a Conditioned Suppression Task with Humans. Learning and Motivation, 39, 114-135. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2007.09.003
- Pavlov, I.P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford University Press.
- Pearce, J.M. (1994). Similarity and discrimination: a selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587–607. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.587
- Rescorla, R. A. (1979). Conditioned inhibition and extinction. In A Dickinson & R. A. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanisms oflearning and motivation: A memorial volume to Jerzy Konorski (pp. 83-110). Erlbaum
- Rescorla, R. A., & Heth, C. D. (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 88–96. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0097-7403.1.1.88
- Vervliet, B., Craske, M. G., & Hermans, D. (2013). Fear extinction and relapse: State of art. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 9, 215-248. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-050212-185542
- Vila, N. J., & Rosas, J. M. (2001). Reinstatement of acquisition performance by the presentation of the outcome after extinction in causality judgements. Behavioural Processes, 56, 147–154. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0376-6357(01)00197-8
- Westbrook, R.F., Iordanova, M., McNally, G., Richardson, R., & Harris, J.A. (2002). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus: two roles for context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 28, 97–110. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037//0097-7403.28.1.97