Executive Functions and Self- Esteem in Academic PerformanceA Mediational Analysis

  1. Quílez-Robres, Alberto 1
  2. Moyano, Nieves 2
  3. Cortés-Pascual, Alejandra 3
  1. 1 Grupo de Investigación EDUCAVIVA, Departamento de Ciencias de la Educación, Facultad de Ciencias Humanas y de la Educación, Universidad de Zaragoza.
  2. 2 Departamento de Psicología, Facultad de Humanidades y Ciencias de la Educación, Universidad de Jaén.
  3. 3 Grupo de Investigación EDUCAVIVA, Departamento de Ciencias de la Educación, Facultad de Educación, Universidad de Zaragoza.
Revista:
International Journal of Psychological Research

ISSN: 2011-7922 2011-2084

Any de publicació: 2021

Volum: 14

Número: 2

Pàgines: 52-60

Tipus: Article

DOI: 10.21500/20112084.5198 DIALNET GOOGLE SCHOLAR lock_openDialnet editor

Altres publicacions en: International Journal of Psychological Research

Objectius de Desenvolupament Sostenible

Resum

Objective: Once the paradigm of intelligence as the only predictor of academic performance has been overcome, the influence of other variables, such as reasoning, verbal fluency, executive functions, motivation and self-esteem, was studied. Method: For this purpose, an exploratory and incidental research design was used in a sample of 132 subjects aged 6-9 years. Different instruments were administered: RAVEN, Effective Reading, Brief II, MAPE II, and Coopersmith Scale, respectively. Results: The results indicate that the predictive model formed by reasoning, verbal fluency, executive functions, and self-esteem explains 55.4% of the academic results. As mediating variables, self-esteem emerges as a predictor of both cognitive and motivational variables, and executive functions, as a predictor of emotional and motivational variables. Discussion: This implies theoretical and practical implications of an educational nature with practical implications in primary school classrooms, in order to implement plans to develop self-esteem and executive functions.

Referències bibliogràfiques

  • Aadland, K. N., Ommundsen, Y., Aadland, E., Bronnick, K. S., Lervag, A., Resaland, G. K., & Moe, V. F. (2017). Executive functions do not mediate prospective relations between indices of physical activity and academic performance: the Active Smarter Kids (ASK) study. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1088. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01088.
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21 (4), 327–336. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.
  • Birkeland, M. S., Melkevik, O., Holsen, I., & Wold, B. (2012). Trajectories of global self-esteem development during adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 35 (1), 43–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.06.006.
  • Bruijn, A. G. M., Hartman, E., Kostons, D., Visscher, C., & Bosker, R. J. (2018). Exploring the relations among physical fitness, executive functioning, and low academic achievement. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 204–221. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.10.010.
  • Bryce, D., Whitebread, D., & Szucs, D. (2015). The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievment in 5 and 7 year-ols children. Metacognition and Learning, 10, 181–198. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4.
  • Caro, D., & Kyriakides, L. (2019). Assessment design and quality of inferences in PISA: Limitations and recommendations for improvement. Journal Assessment in Education: Principles, Policy & Practice, 26, 363–368. https://doi.org/10.1080/0969594X.2019.1645990.
  • Colom, R., Rubio, V. J., Shih, P. C., & Santacreu, J. (2006). Fluid intelligence, working memory and executive functioning. Psicothema, 18 (4), 816-821. https://reunido.uniovi.es/index.php/PST/article/view/8382.
  • Coopersmith, S. (1967). The antecedents of self-esteem. Freeman & Company.
  • Cortés Pascual, A., Moyano Muñoz, N., & Quilez Robres, A. (2019). The Relationship Between Executive Functions and Academic Performance in Primary Education: Review and Meta-Analysis. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 1582. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01582.
  • Davis, E. R. (2019). The PISA Phenomenon: Analysis of its Ascendancy in Media, Policymaker and Academic Discourses in Education [Tesis Doctoral. Universidad de Sydney]. https://ses.library.usyd.edu.au/handle/2123/21469.
  • de Castro, R. M., & Pereira, D. I. F. (2019). Education and Attachment: Guidelines to Prevent School Failure. Multimodal Technologies and Interaction, 3 (1), 10. https://doi.org/10.3390/mti3010010.
  • Evans, J. S. (2003). In two minds: Dual–process accounts of reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciencies, 7, 454–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2003.08.012.
  • Fernández-Mellizo, M., & Martínez-García, J. S. (2017). Inequality of educational opportunities: School failure trends in Spain (1977–2012). International Studies in Sociology of Education, 26 (3), 267–287. https://doi.org/10.1080/09620214.2016.1192954.
  • Floyd, R. G., McGrew, K. S., & Evans, J. J. (2008). The relative contributions of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll cognitive abilities in explaining writing achievement during childhood and adolescence. Psychology in the Schools, 45 (2), 132–144. https://doi.org/10.1002/pits.20284.
  • Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2017). In M. J. Maldonado, M. C. Fournier, R. Martínez-Arias, J. González-Marqués, J. M. Espejo-Saavedra, & P. Santamaría (adapters), BRIEF-2. Evaluación Conductual de la Función Ejecutiva. TEA Ediciones.
  • Greene, M. L., &Way, N. (2005). Selfesteem trajectories among ethnic minority adolescents: A growth curve analysis of the patterns and predictors of change. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 15 (2), 151–178. https://doi.org/ 10.1111/j.1532-7795.2005.00090.x.
  • Grupo Editorial Bruño. (2017). Lectura eficaz – juegos de lectura. Editorial Bruño, S.L.
  • Harter, S., & Whitesell, N. R. (2003). Beyond the debate: Whysome adolescents report stable self-worth. Journal of Personality, 71 (6), 1027–1058. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-6494.7106006.
  • Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling. https://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf.
  • Jacobson, L. A., Williford, A. P., & Pianta, R. C. (2011). The role of executive function in children’s competent adjustment to middle school. Child Neuropsychology, 17 (3), 255–280. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.535654.
  • Kaplan, D. S., Peck, M., & Kaplan, H. B. (1994). Structural relations model of self–rejection, disposition to deviance, and academic failure. Journal of Educational Research, 87 (3), 166–173. https://doi.org/10.1080/00220671.1994.9941238.
  • Kiviruusu, O., Konttinen, H., Huurre, T., Aro, H., Marttunen, M., & Haukkala, A. (2016). Self-esteem and body mass index from adolescence to mid-adulthood. a 26-year follow-up. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 23 (3), 355–363. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-015-9529-4.
  • Lamas, H. A. (2015). Sobre el rendimiento escolar. Propósitos y representaciones, 3 (1), 313–386. https://doi.org/10.20511/pyr2015.v3n1.74.
  • Masselink, M., Van Roekel, E., & Oldehinkel, A. J. (2018). Self–esteem in early adolescence as predictor of depressive symptoms in late adolescence and early adulthood: the mediating role of motivational and social factors. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47 (5), 932–946. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-017-0727-z.
  • McArthur, G., Castles, A., Khonen, S., & Banales, E. (2016). Low self-concept in por readers: Prevalence, heterogeneity, and risk. PeerJ, 4, 1–25. https://doi.or/10.7717/peerj.2669.
  • Mercader, J., Presentación, M. J., Siegenthaler, R., Molinero, V., & Miranda, A. (2017). Motivación y rendimiento académico en matemáticas: Un estudio longitudinal en las primeras etapas educativas. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 22 (2), 157–163. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psicod.2017.05.007.
  • Miranda Esquer, J. B., Miranda Esquer, J. F., & Enríquez Valdenebro, A. L. (2011). Adaptación del Inventario de Autoestima Coopersmith para alumnos mexicanos de educación primaria. Praxis investigativa ReDIE: Revista Electrónica de la Red Durango de Investigadores Educativos, 3 (4), 5–14. https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=6535715.
  • Montero, I., & Alonso, J. (1992). Validez predictiva de los cuestionarios MAPE-II y EMA-II. En J. Alonso Tapia (Dir.), Motivar en la adolescencia: Teoría, Evaluación e Intervención (pp. 263-280). Ediciones de la Universidad Autónoma.
  • Moyano, N., Quílez-Robres, A., & Cortés Pascual, A. (2020). Self-esteem and motivation for learning in academic achievement: The mediating role of reasoning and verbal fluidity. Sustainability, 12 (14), 5768. https://doi.org/10.3390/su12145768.
  • Mulder, H., Verhagen, J., Van der Ven, S. H. G., Slot, P. L., & Leseman, P. P. M. (2017). Early executive function at age two predicts emergent mathematics and literacy at age five. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 1706. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01706.
  • Nascimento, S., & Peixoto, F. (2012). Relações entre o estatuto escolar e o autoconceito, auto–estima e orientações motivacionais em alunos do 9◦ ano de escolaridade. Análise Psicológica, 30 (4), 421–434. http://www.scielo.mec.pt/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S0870-82312012000300005.
  • Nie, J., Zhang, W., & Liu, Y. (2017). Exploring depression, self–esteem and verbal fluency with different degrees of internet addiction among Chinese college students. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 72, 114–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2016.10.006.
  • Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 879–891. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.40.3.879.
  • Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2010). Test de matrices progresivas de raven. Pearson-Anaya. Regueiro, B., Suárez, N., Valle, A., Núñez, J. C., & Rosário, P. (2015). La motivación e implicación en los deberes escolares a lo largo de la escolaridad obligatoria. Revista de Psicodidáctica, 20 (1), 47–63. https://doi.org/10.1387/RevPsicodidact.126411.
  • Ren, X., Schweizer, K., Wang, T., & Xu, F. (2015). The prediction of students’ academic performance with fluid intelligence in giving special consideration to the contribution of learning. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 11 (3), 97–105. https://doi.org/10.5709/acp-0175-z.
  • Rhoades, B. L., Warren, H. K., Domitrovich, C. E., & Greenberg, M. T. (2011). Examining the link between preschool social–emotional competence and first grade academic achievement: the role of attention skills. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 26, 182–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.07.003.
  • Schneider, W. J., & McGrew, K. S. (2012). The Cattell–Horn–Carroll model of intelligence. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues( p. 99144). The Guilford Press.
  • St. Clair-Thompson, H. L., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition and working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59 (4), 745–759. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210500162854.
  • Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E., Ferrando, M., Hernández, D., Ferrándiz, C., & Bermejo, R. (2010). Enseñanza de la inteligencia exitosa para alumnos superdotados y talentos. Revista Electrónica Interuniversitaria de Formación del Profesorado, 13 (1), 111–118. https://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/articulo?codigo=3163513.
  • Walters, G. D., & Mandracchia, J. T. (2017). Testing criminological theory through causal mediation analysis: Current status and future directions. Journal of Criminal Justice, 49, 53–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2017.02.002.
  • Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor análisis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44 (2), 575–587. https://doi.or/10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575.
  • Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., & Greenberg, M. (2010). The measurement of executive function at age 3 years: Psychometric properties and criterion validity of a new battery of tasks. Psychological Assessment, 22( (2), 306–317. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018708.
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development. Perspectives, 6, 354–360. https//doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2012.00246.x.