Perspectivas de atención en dependencia del contexto de recuperación de información
- Pedro M Ogallar
- Manuel M Ramos Álvarez
- José A Alcalá
- María M Moreno Fernández
- Juan M Rosas
ISSN: 1577-7057
Año de publicación: 2017
Volumen: 17
Número: 1
Páginas: 121-136
Tipo: Artículo
Otras publicaciones en: International journal of psychology and psychological therapy
Resumen
La atención se ha entendido tradicionalmente como un factor importante en la adquisición de nueva información. Una revisión de la literatura sugiere que la atención, específicamente la atención a los contextos, también juega un papel relevante en la recuperación de información. También demuestra que la atención a los contextos es modulada por la ambigüedad de la situación, y los contextos valor informativo tiene. Se discuten las virtudes y limitaciones de las diferentes teorías atencionales de aprendizaje aplicado a la explicación de los efectos del cambio de contexto en la recuperación de la información. Este análisis revela las debilidades de la investigación actual sobre el procesamiento de contexto que deben ser corregidos por la investigación futura: La necesidad de medidas independientes de la atención a los contextos, la evaluación de los mecanismos de control contextual, y la posibilidad de tomar una perspectiva evolutiva sobre los efectos de cambio de contexto
Referencias bibliográficas
- Abad MJF, Ramos Álvarez MM, & Rosas JM (2009). Partial reinforcement and context switch effects in human predictive learning. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 174-199. Doi: 10.1080/17470210701855561
- Amsel A (1958). The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuos reward situations. Psychological Bulletin, 55, 102-119. Doi: 10.1037/h0043125
- Amsel A (1992). Frustration Theory. Canada: Cambridge University Press. Doi: 10.1017/cbo9780511665561
- Anderson JR (1993). Rules of the Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Doi: 10.4324/9781315806938
- Aristizabal JA, Ramos Álvarez MM, Callejas Aguilera JE, & Rosas JM (2016). Attention to irrelevant contexts decreases as training increases: Evidence from eye-fixations in a human predictive learning task. Behavioural Processes, 124, 66-73. Doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.12.008
- Aristizabal JA, Ramos Álvarez MM, Callejas Aguilera JE, & Rosas JM (2017). Context-switches after short acquisition lead to an increase on attention to the context in a human predictive learning task. Unpublished Manuscript. Universidad de Jaén.
- Bernal Gamboa R, Callejas Aguilera JE, Nieto J, & Rosas, JM (2013). Extinction makes conditioning time-dependent. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39, 221-232. Doi: 10.1037/a0032181
- Bernal Gamboa R, Nieto J, & Rosas JM (2015). Context specificity of taste aversion is boosted by pre-exposure and conditioning with a different taste. Behavioural Processes, 120, 111-115. Doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2015.09.008
- Bernal Gamboa R, Rosas JM, & Callejas Aguilera, JE (2014). Experiencing extinction within a task makes nonextinguished information learned within a different task context-dependent. Psychonomic Bulletin Review, 21, 803-808. Doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0558-1
- Bonardi C, Honey RC, & Hall G (1990). Context specificity of conditioning in flavor-aversion learning: Extinction and blocking tests. Animal Learning & Behavior, 18, 229-237. Doi: 10.3758/bf03205280
- Bouton ME (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 80-99. Doi: 10.1037//0033-2909.114.1.80
- Bouton ME (1994). Conditioning, remembering, and forgetting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 20, 219-231. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.20.3.219
- Bouton ME (1997). Signals for whether versus when an event will occur. In ME Bouton & MS Fanselow (Eds.), Learning, motivation and cognition: The functional behaviourism of Robert C. Bolles (pp. 385-409). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
- Bouton ME & Bolles RC (1979). Contextual control of the extinction of conditioned fear. Learning and Motivation, 10, 445-466. Doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(79)90057-2
- Bouton ME & Ricker ST (1994). Renewal of extinguished responding in a second context. Animal Learning & Behavior, 22, 317-324. Doi: 10.3758/bf03209840
- Bouton ME & Swartzentruber D (1986). Analysis of the associative and occasion-setting properties of contexts participating in a Pavlovian discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 12, 333-350. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.12.4.333
- Callejas Aguilera JE, Cubillas CP, & Rosas JM (2017). Attentional instructions modulate differential context-switch effects after short and long training in human predictive learning. Unpublished Manuscript. Universidad de Jaén.
- Callejas Aguilera JE & Rosas JM (2010). Ambiguity and context processing in human predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 36, 482-494. Doi: 10.1037/a0018527
- Capaldi EJ (1967). A sequential hypothesis of instrumental learning. In KW Spence & JT Spence (Eds.), The psychology of learning and motivation, Volume 1 (pp. 67-156). New York: Academic Press.
- Capaldi EJ (1994). The relation between memory and expectancy as revealed by percentage and sequence of reward investigations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1, 303-310. Doi: 10.3758/BF03213970
- Darby RJ & Pearce J (1995). Effects of context on responding during a compound stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 21, 143-154. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.21.2.143
- Delamater AR (1997). Selective reinstatement of stimulus-outcome associations. Animal Learning & Behavior, 25, 400-412. Doi: 10.3758/bf03209847
- Deroost N & Soetens E (2006). Spatial processing and perceptual sequence learning in SRT tasks. Experimental Psychology, 53, 16-30. Doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.1.16
- Dickinson A, Hall G, & Mackintosh NJ (1976). Surprise and the attenuation of blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 313-322. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.2.4.313
- Eich E (1985). Context, memory, and integrated item/context imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 11, 764-770. Doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.11.1-4.764
- Gámez AM, León SP, & Rosas JM (2016). Roles of context in acquisition of human instrumental learning: Implications for the understanding of the mechanisms underlying context-switch effects. Learning & Behavior. Doi: 10.3758/s13420-016-0256-8
- García Gutiérrez A, Rosas JM, & Nelson JB (2005). Extensive interference attenuates reinstatement in human predictive judgments. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 18, 240-248.
- García Gutiérrez A & Rosas JM (2003). Context change as the mechanism of reinstatement in causal learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 29, 292-310. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.29.4.292
- Griffiths O & Mitchell CJ (2008). Selective attention in human associative learning and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 626-648. Doi: 10.1037/a0013685
- Hall G & Pearce JM (1979). Latent inhibition of a CS during CS-US pairings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 5, 31-42. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.5.1.31
- Hall G & Honey RC (1990). Context-specific conditioning in the conditioned-emotional-response procedure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 16, 271-278. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.16.3.271
- Hall G & Rodríguez G (2010). Attentional learning. In C Mitchell & M Le Pelley (Eds.), Attention and associative learning (pp. 41-70). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
- Harris JA, Jones ML, Bailey GK & Westbrook RF (2000). Contextual control over conditioned responding in an extinction paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26, 174-185. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.26.2.174
- Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Janowski M, Nikitina A, & Duka, T (2008). The role of attentional bias in mediating human drug-seeking behavior. Psychopharmacology, 201, 29-41. Doi: 10.1007/s00213-008-1244-2
- Hogarth L, Dickinson A, Wright A, Kouvaraki M, & Duka, T (2007). The role of drug expectancy in the control of human drug seeking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 484-496. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.33.4.484
- Kamin LJ (1969). Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning. In B Campbell & R Church (Eds.), Punishment and aversive behavior (pp. 279-298). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
- Kaye H & Pearce JM (1984). The strength of the orienting response during Pavlovian conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 10, 90-109. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.10.1.90
- Keene CS & Bucci DJ (2007). Automated measure of conditioned orienting behavior in rats. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 303-308. Doi: 10.3758/bf03193161
- Krechevsky I (1932). Hypotheses in rats. Psychological Review, 39, 516-532. Doi: 10.1037/h0073500
- Kruschke JK (2001). Toward a unified model of attention in associative learning. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 45, 812-863. Doi: 10.1006/jmps.2000.1354
- Kruschke JK (2003). Attention in learning. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 171-175. Doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01254
- Le Pelley ME (2004). The role of associative history in models of associative learning: A selective review and a hybrid model. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57B, 193-243. Doi: 10.1080/02724990344000141
- Le Pelley M, Mitchell C, Beesley T, George D, & Wills AJ (2016). Attention and associative learning in humans: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 142, 1111-1140. Doi: 10.1037/bul0000064
- Le Pelley ME, Vadillo M, & Luque D (2013). Learned predictiveness influences rapid attentional capture: Evidence from the dot probe task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 18881900. https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/a0033700
- León SP, Abad MJF, & Rosas JM (2008). Retrieval of simple cue-outcome relationships is context-specific within informative contexts. Escritos de Psicología, 2, 63-71.
- León SP, Abad MJF, & Rosas JM (2010a). Giving contexts informative value makes information context specific. Experimental Psychology, 57, 46-53. Doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000006
- León SP, Abad MJF, & Rosas JM (2010b). The effect of context change on simple acquisition disappears with increased training. Psicologica, 31, 49-63.
- León SP, Abad MJF, & Rosas JM (2011). Context-outcome associations mediate context-switch effects in a human predictive learning task. Learning and Motivation, 42, 84-98. Doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2010.10.001
- León SP, Callejas-Aguilera JE, & Rosas JM (2012). Context switch effects and context experience in rats’ conditioned taste aversion. Psicologica, 33, 15-38.
- León SP, Gámez MA, & Rosas JM (2012). Mechanisms of contextual control when contexts are informative to solve the task. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15, 10-19. Doi: 10.5209/rev_sjop.2012.v15.n1.37279
- Lucke S, Lachnit H, Koenig S, & Uengoer, M. (2013). The informational value of contexts affects context-dependent learning. Learning & Behavior, 41, 285–97. Doi: 10.3758/s13420-013-0104-z
- Lucke S, Lachnit H, Stüttgen MC, & Uengoer M (2014). The impact of context relevance during extinction learning. Learning & Behavior, 42, 256–269. Doi: 10.3758/s13420-014-0143-0
- Luque D, Vadillo MA, Le Pelley ME, & Beesley T (2016). Prediction and uncertainty in associative learning: Examining controlled and automatic components of learned attentional biases. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 13, 1-52. Doi: 10.1080/17470218.2016.1188407
- Mackintosh NJ (1975). A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement. Psychological Review, 82, 276-298. Doi: 10.1037/h0076778
- Mackintosh NJ (1975). Blocking of conditioned suppression: Role of the first compound trial. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 335-345. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.1.4.335
- Mensink GJ & Raaijmakers JG (1988). A model for interference and forgetting. Psychological Review, 95, 434-455. Doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.95.4.434
- Murphy R, Baker A, & Fouquet N (2001). Relative validity of contextual and discrete cues. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2, 137-152. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.27.2.137
- Myers CE & Gluck MA (1994). Context, conditioning and hippocampal re-representation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 108, 835-847. Doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.108.5.835
- Nakajima S, Tanaka S, Urushihara K, & Imada H (2000). Renewal of extinguished lever-press responses upon return to the training context. Learning and Motivation, 31, 416-431. Doi: 10.1006/lmot.2000.1064
- Nelson JB (2002). Context specificity of excitation and inhibition in ambiguous stimuli. Learning and Motivation, 33, 284-310. Doi: 10.1006/lmot.2001.1112
- Nelson JB & Callejas-Aguilera JE (2007). The role of interference produced by conflicting associations in contextual control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3, 314-326. Doi: 10.1037/00977403.33.3.314
- Nelson JB & Lamoureux JA (2015). Contextual control of conditioning is not affected by extinction in a behavioral task with humans. Learning & Behavior, 43, 163-178. Doi:10.3758/s13420-015-0170-5
- Nelson JB, Lamoreux JA, & León SP (2013). Extinction arouses attention to the context in a behavioral suppression method with humans. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 39, 99-105. Doi: 10.1037/a0030759
- Nelson JB, Lombas S, & Léon, SP (2011). Concurrent extinction does not render appetitive conditioning context specific. Learning & Behavior, 39, 87-94. Doi: 10.3758/s13420-011-0023-9
- Nelson JB, Sanjuan MC, Vadillo-Ruiz S, Pérez J, & León SP (2011). Experimental Renewal in Human Participants, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 37, 58-70. Doi: 10.1037/a0020519
- Neumann DL (2007). The resistance of renewal to instructions that devalue the role of contextual cues in a conditioned suppression task with humans. Learning and Motivation, 38, 105-127. Doi: 10.1016/j.lmot.2006.11.002
- Ostlund SB & Balleine BW (2007) Selective reinstatement of instrumental performance depends on the discriminative stimulus properties of the mediating outcome. Learning & Behavior, 35, 43–52. Doi: 10.3758/bf03196073
- Paredes Olay C & Rosas JM (1999). Within-subjects extinction and renewal in predictive judgments. Psicologica, 20, 195-210.
- Pavlov IP (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press. Pearce JM (1987). A model of stimulus generalization for Pavlovian conditioning. Psychological Review, 94, 61-73. Doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.94.1.61
- Pearce JM (1994). Similarity and discrimination: A selective review and a connectionist model. Psychological Review, 101, 587-607. Doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.101.4.587
- Pearce JM (2002). Evaluation and development of a connectionist theory of configural learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 30, 73-95. Doi: 10.3758/bf03192911
- Pearce JM & Hall G (1980). A model for Pavlovian learning: Variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychological Review, 87, 532-552. Doi: 10.1037//0033-295x.87.6.532
- Pearce JM & Mackintosh NJ (2010). Two theories of attention: A review and a possible integration. In CJ Mitchell & ME Le Pelley (Eds.), Attention and Associative Learning: from Brain to Behaviour (pp. 11-40), New York: Oxford University Press.
- Pearce JM, Redhead ES, & Aydin A (1997). Partial reinforcement in appetitive Pavlovian conditioning with rats. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50B, 273-294.
- Posner MI, Nissen MJ, & Ogden WC (1978) Attended and unattended processing modes: The role of set for spatial location. In HL Pick & IJ Saltzman (Eds.) Modes of perceiving and processing information (pp. 137-157). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Preston GC, Dickinson A, & Mackintosh NJ (1986). Contextual conditional discriminations. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 38B, 217-237.
- Rescorla RA & Heth CD (1975). Reinstatement of fear to an extinguished conditioned stimulus. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1, 88-96. Doi: 10.1037//0097-7403.1.1.88
- Rescorla R & Wagner A (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In AH Black & WF Prokasy (Eds.), Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory (pp. 64-99). New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
- Rosas JM, Callejas Aguilera JE, Ramos Álvarez MM, & Fernández Abad MJ (2006). Revision of Retrieval Theory of Forgetting: What does Make Information Context-Specific? International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 6, 147-166.
- Rosas JM, García Gutiérrez A, & Callejas Aguilera, JE (2006). Effects of context change upon first and second-learned information in human predictive learning. Psicologica, 27, 35-56.
- Rosas JM, García Gutiérrez A, & Callejas Aguilera JE (2007). AAB and ABA renewal as a function of the number of extinction trials in conditioned taste aversion. Psicologica, 28, 129-150.
- Rosas JM, Todd TP, & Bouton ME (2013). Context change and associative learning. WIRE Cognitive Science, 4, 237-244. Doi: 10.1002/wcs.1225
- Rosas JM, Vila NJ, Lugo M, & López L (2001). Combined effect of context change and retention interval upon interference in causality judgments. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 27, 153-164. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.27.2.153
- Rosas JM & Callejas Aguilera JE (2006). Context switch effects on acquisition and extinction in human predictive learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 32, 461-474. Doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.461
- Rosas JM & Callejas Aguilera JE (2007). Acquisition of a conditioned taste aversion becomes context dependent when it is learned alter extinction. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 9-15. Doi: 10.1080/17470210600971519
- Schmajuk NA, Lam Y, & Gray JA (1996). Latent inhibition: A neural network approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 22, 321-349. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.22.3.321
- Sjödén P & Archer T (1989). Taste-aversion conditioning: The role of contextual stimuli. In T Archer & LG Nilsson (Eds.), Aversion, avoidance, and anxiety perspectives on aversively motivated behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
- Sutherland NS & Mackintosh NJ (1971). Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning. New York: Academic Press.
- Swan JA & Pearce JM (1988). The orienting response as an index of stimulus associability in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 14, 292-301. Doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.14.3.292
- Tamai N & Nakajima S (2000). Renewal of formerly conditioned fear in rats after extensive extinction training. International Journal of Comparative Psychology 13, 137-147.
- Thomas BL, Larsen NL, & Ayres JJB (2003). Role of context similarity in ABA, ABC, and AAB renewal paradigms: Implications for theories of renewal and for treating human phobias. Learning and Motivation, 34, 410-436. Doi: 10.1016/s0023-9690(03)00037-7
- Vadillo MA, Orgaz C, Luque D, & Nelson JB (2016). Ambiguity produces attention shifts in category learning. Learning & Memory, 23, 134-140. Doi: 10.1101/lm.041145.115
- Wagner AR & Rescorla RA (1972). Inhibition in Pavlovian conditioning: Application of a theory. In RA Boakes & MS Halliday (Eds.), Inhibition and Learning (pp. 301-336). New York: Academic Press.