Contextual Control and Generalization of Say-Do Correspondence. A Preliminary Study
- Hernández López, Mónica
- Rodríguez Valverde, Miguel
- Luciano Soriano, María Carmen
ISSN: 1577-7057
Year of publication: 2011
Volume: 11
Issue: 2
Pages: 269-284
Type: Article
More publications in: International journal of psychology and psychological therapy
Abstract
El propósito de este estudio es analizar las condiciones que permiten el control contextual de la correspondencia decir-hacer y su generalización a estímulos bajo los cuales la correspondencia no ha sido explícitamente entrenada. Ocho niños de entre cinco y seis años de edad y con un desarrollo normal participaron en el experimento. Dos conjuntos de estímulos, familiares y fácilmente reconocibles, fueron utilizados como claves contextuales para el entrenamiento de la correspondencia y la no correspondencia. Se entrenó a los niños para hacer lo que previamente habían dicho (correspondencia decir-hacer) ante tres elementos de uno de los conjuntos, y para hacer algo diferente de lo que habían dicho (no-correspondencia) ante tres elementos del otro conjunto; para ello se utilizaron instrucciones y se presentaron contingencias diferenciales por la correspondencia y la no-correspondencia. Por último se evaluó la función discriminativa para la correspondencia y la no-correspondencia de los dos elementos restantes de cada conjunto. Los resultados mostraron la generalización de las dos formas de relación decir-hacer en seis de los ocho participantes. En la mayoría de los ensayos de prueba los participantes mostraron la relación decir-hacer esperada con secuencias decir-hacer específicas que no habían sido previamente entrenadas.
Bibliographic References
- Baer RA & Detrich R (1990). Tacting and manding in correspondence training: effects of child selection of verbalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 23-30.
- Baer DM, Peterson RF, & Sherman JA (1967). The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 405-416.
- Bevill-Davis A, Cless TJ, & Gast DL (2004). Correspondence training: a review of the literature. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 1, 14-27.
- de Freitas Ribeiro A (1989). Correspondence in children’s self-report: Tacting and manding aspects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 361-367.
- Deacon JR & Konarski EA (1987). Correspondence training: an example of rule-governed behavior? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 391-400.
- Guevremont DC, Osnes PG, & Stokes TF (1986). Preparation for effective self-regulation: the development of generalized verbal control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 99-104.
- Hayes SC, Barnes-Holmes D, & Roche B (2001). Relational Frame Theory. A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Languaje and Cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
- Herruzo J & Luciano MC (1994). Procedimientos para establecer la “correspondencia decir-hacer”. Un análisis de sus elementos y problemas pendientes. Acta Comportamentalia, 2, 192-218.
- Israel AC & O’Leary KD (1973). Developing correspondence between children’s words and deeds. Child Development, 44, 575-581.
- Lloyd KE (2002). A review of correspondence training: suggestion for a revival. The Behavior Analyst, 25, 57-73.
- Luciano MC, Barnes-Holmes Y, & Barnes-Holmes D (2002). Establishing reports of saying and doing and discriminations of say-do relations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, 406-421.
- Luciano MC, Herruzo J, & Barnes-Holmes D (2001). Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111-130.
- Luciano MC, Molina Cobos FJ, & Gómez Becerra I (2000). Say-do-report training to change chronic behaviors in mentally retarded subjects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 355-366.
- Luciano MC, Vílchez F, & Herruzo J (1992). Say-do and thumbsucking behavior. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 14, 63-69.
- Matthews BA, Shimoff E, & Catania AC (1987). Saying and doing: a contingency-space analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 69-74.
- Molina Cobos FJ, Amador Castro MC, & Fernández Rodríguez MD (2008). Correspondencia decir-hacer para la mejora de conductas perturbadoras en adultos con síndrome de Down. Psicothema, 20, 71-79.
- Osnes PG, Guevremont DC, & Stokes TF (1987). Increasing a child’s prosocial behaviors: positive and negative consequences in correspondence training. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 71-76.
- Paniagua FA (1985). Development of self-care skills and helping behaviors of adolescents in a group home through correspondence training. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 16, 237-244.
- Paniagua FA & Black S (1990). Management and prevention of hyperactivity and conduct disorders in 8-10 year-old boys through correspondence training procedures. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 12, 23-56.
- Paniagua FA, Pumariega AJ, & Black SA (1988). Clinical effects of correspondence training in the management of hyperactive children. Behavioral Interventions, 3, 19-40.
- Risley TR & Hart B (1968). Developing correspondence between the non-verbal and verbal behavior of preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 267-281.
- Rogers-Warren AR & Baer DM (1976). Correspondence between saying and doing: teaching children to share and praise. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 335-354.
- Stokes JV, Cameron MJ, Dorsey MF, & Fleming E (2004). Task analysis, correspondence training, and general case instruction for teaching personal hygiene skills. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 121-135.
- Stokes TF, Osnes PG, & Guevremont DC (1987). Saying and doing: A commentary on a contingencyspace analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 161-164.
- Sidman M & Tailby W (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expasion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22.
- Whitman TL, Scibak JW, Butler KM, Richter R, & Johnson MR (1982). Improving classroom behavior in mentally retarded children through correspondence training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 545-564.