Contextual Control and Generalization of Say-Do Correspondence. A Preliminary Study

  1. Hernández López, Mónica
  2. Rodríguez Valverde, Miguel
  3. Luciano Soriano, María Carmen
Journal:
International journal of psychology and psychological therapy

ISSN: 1577-7057

Year of publication: 2011

Volume: 11

Issue: 2

Pages: 269-284

Type: Article

More publications in: International journal of psychology and psychological therapy

Abstract

The purpose of this study is to analyze the conditions that permit the contextual control of correspondence and its generalization to stimuli under which correspondence was not explicitly trained. Eight typically developing preschool children participated in the experiment. Two sets of five easily recognizable and familiar stimuli were used as contextual cues for correspondence and non-correspondence training. Using instructions and differential contingencies for correspondence and non-correspondence, children were trained to do what they previously had said (say-do correspondence) with three elements of one set, and to do something different to what they previously had said (non-correspondence) with three elements of the other set. The remaining two elements of each set were then tested for their function as discriminative-like stimuli for either correspondence or non-correspondence. Results showed generalization of the two different forms of say-do relations for six participants out of eight. In most test trials, participants showed adequate correspondence or non-correspondence with untrained say-do sequences

Bibliographic References

  • Baer RA & Detrich R (1990). Tacting and manding in correspondence training: effects of child selection of verbalization. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 23-30.
  • Baer DM, Peterson RF, & Sherman JA (1967). The development of imitation by reinforcing behavioral similarity to a model. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 405-416.
  • Bevill-Davis A, Cless TJ, & Gast DL (2004). Correspondence training: a review of the literature. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 1, 14-27.
  • de Freitas Ribeiro A (1989). Correspondence in children’s self-report: Tacting and manding aspects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 361-367.
  • Deacon JR & Konarski EA (1987). Correspondence training: an example of rule-governed behavior? Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 391-400.
  • Guevremont DC, Osnes PG, & Stokes TF (1986). Preparation for effective self-regulation: the development of generalized verbal control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 19, 99-104.
  • Hayes SC, Barnes-Holmes D, & Roche B (2001). Relational Frame Theory. A Post-Skinnerian Account of Human Languaje and Cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers.
  • Herruzo J & Luciano MC (1994). Procedimientos para establecer la “correspondencia decir-hacer”. Un análisis de sus elementos y problemas pendientes. Acta Comportamentalia, 2, 192-218.
  • Israel AC & O’Leary KD (1973). Developing correspondence between children’s words and deeds. Child Development, 44, 575-581.
  • Lloyd KE (2002). A review of correspondence training: suggestion for a revival. The Behavior Analyst, 25, 57-73.
  • Luciano MC, Barnes-Holmes Y, & Barnes-Holmes D (2002). Establishing reports of saying and doing and discriminations of say-do relations. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 23, 406-421.
  • Luciano MC, Herruzo J, & Barnes-Holmes D (2001). Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111-130.
  • Luciano MC, Molina Cobos FJ, & Gómez Becerra I (2000). Say-do-report training to change chronic behaviors in mentally retarded subjects. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 21, 355-366.
  • Luciano MC, Vílchez F, & Herruzo J (1992). Say-do and thumbsucking behavior. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 14, 63-69.
  • Matthews BA, Shimoff E, & Catania AC (1987). Saying and doing: a contingency-space analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 69-74.
  • Molina Cobos FJ, Amador Castro MC, & Fernández Rodríguez MD (2008). Correspondencia decir-hacer para la mejora de conductas perturbadoras en adultos con síndrome de Down. Psicothema, 20, 71-79.
  • Osnes PG, Guevremont DC, & Stokes TF (1987). Increasing a child’s prosocial behaviors: positive and negative consequences in correspondence training. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 18, 71-76.
  • Paniagua FA (1985). Development of self-care skills and helping behaviors of adolescents in a group home through correspondence training. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 16, 237-244.
  • Paniagua FA & Black S (1990). Management and prevention of hyperactivity and conduct disorders in 8-10 year-old boys through correspondence training procedures. Child and Family Behavior Therapy, 12, 23-56.
  • Paniagua FA, Pumariega AJ, & Black SA (1988). Clinical effects of correspondence training in the management of hyperactive children. Behavioral Interventions, 3, 19-40.
  • Risley TR & Hart B (1968). Developing correspondence between the non-verbal and verbal behavior of preschool children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1, 267-281.
  • Rogers-Warren AR & Baer DM (1976). Correspondence between saying and doing: teaching children to share and praise. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 335-354.
  • Stokes JV, Cameron MJ, Dorsey MF, & Fleming E (2004). Task analysis, correspondence training, and general case instruction for teaching personal hygiene skills. Behavioral Interventions, 19, 121-135.
  • Stokes TF, Osnes PG, & Guevremont DC (1987). Saying and doing: A commentary on a contingencyspace analysis. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 20, 161-164.
  • Sidman M & Tailby W (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expasion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22.
  • Whitman TL, Scibak JW, Butler KM, Richter R, & Johnson MR (1982). Improving classroom behavior in mentally retarded children through correspondence training. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 15, 545-564.